Articles | Volume 15, issue 4
https://doi.org/10.5194/esd-15-969-2024
https://doi.org/10.5194/esd-15-969-2024
Research article
 | 
02 Aug 2024
Research article |  | 02 Aug 2024

Missing the (tipping) point: the effect of information about climate tipping points on public risk perceptions in Norway

Christina Nadeau, Manjana Milkoreit, Thomas Hylland Eriksen, and Dag Olav Hessen
Abstract

Climate tipping points are a topic of growing interest in climate research and a frequent communication tool in the media to warn of dangerous climate change. Despite indications that several climate tipping points may already be triggered within 1.5 to 2 °C warming above pre-industrial levels, there is limited research about the public understanding of climate tipping points, the effects this knowledge (or lack thereof) may have on perceptions of risk related to climate change, and the corresponding effects on behaviour and public policy support. The emerging scholarship on learning, communication, and risk perceptions related to climate tipping points provides confounding evidence regarding the psychological and behavioural effects of information about climate tipping points. It remains unknown whether and under what conditions this knowledge increases concern, risk perceptions, and action intentions. In this study, we assess the current state of knowledge about climate tipping points among Norwegians using an online survey. We study the comparative effects of communicating about climate tipping points and climate change more generally on risk perceptions among participants with a survey-embedded experiment. Norway is an interesting case with its fossil-based economy and high level of education. We find that familiarity with climate tipping points is low among Norwegians: only 13 % have good knowledge in the sense that they know an example or characteristic of climate tipping points. Information about tipping points has somewhat stronger, yet overall very small, effects on participants' risk perceptions compared to general information about climate change, moderately increasing concern. We discuss our findings and their implications and suggest directions for further research.

1 Introduction

Efforts to mitigate climate change require urgent attention from both policymakers and the general public (IPCC, 2022). Despite recent progress, such as the acceleration of growth in renewable energy markets (IEA, 2022), global climate action continues to be insufficient to reach international objectives. While future warming projections have narrowed, pathways towards 1.5–2 °C warming do not appear credible without rapid, large-scale transformations of human systems (Kuramochi et al., 2022). Among the many reasons for this inadequate response to the climate challenge (Stoddard, 2021), public risk perceptions have played an important role. Public risk perceptions affect public support for climate policy and action (Bergquist et al., 2022; Drews and van den Bergh, 2016), and public support is a key condition for climate policy adoption, especially at the local scale (Yeganeh et al., 2020). When Lenton et al. (2008) published their seminal paper introducing the concept of climate tipping elements, they argued that “society may be lulled into a false sense of security by smooth projections of global change” (p. 1792), i.e. that dominant conceptions of gradual, linear change might be at least partly to blame for the relatively low levels of concern about climate change and the persistent lack of urgency among policymakers and the public in the face of significant climate risks. This mental model of gradual change is now increasingly challenged by a growing body of scientific evidence for tipping points in the climate system. Here, we investigate whether exposure to knowledge on climate tipping points affects (i.e. increases) public risk perceptions of climate change in the national context of Norway.

Climate tipping points refer to non-linear change dynamics in large components of the Earth system. These so-called tipping elements can undergo state shifts in the sense that a change process that is initially gradual can reach a threshold (i.e. a tipping point), after which self-amplifying feedback mechanisms propel the system rapidly towards an alternative stable state. In many cases, these state shifts are irreversible on human timescales (Armstrong McKay et al., 2022; Lenton et al., 2008; Steffen et al., 2018). There is some evidence that multiple climate tipping points may be triggered within the temperature target range set by the Paris Agreement: an increase of 1.5 °C to well below 2 °C (Armstrong McKay et al., 2022; Schellnhuber et al., 2016; Wunderling et al., 2023). With recent projections indicating that the increase in global average temperatures could exceed 1.5 °C in the 2030s (IPCC, 2021), perhaps even temporarily in this decade (WMO, 2023), climate tipping processes add new arguments for more ambitious climate action. This growing relevance has been reflected in more frequent appearances of climate tipping points in the assessment reports of the IPCC and in growing media coverage warning of dangerous climate change (van der Hel et al., 2018).

However, it is as yet unclear to what extent and how climate tipping points are understood by relevant audiences, how knowledge of climate tipping points affects climate risk perceptions, and whether and how this will influence behaviour or climate policymaking. The growing importance of climate tipping processes as a topic for climate risk communication and action is underexplored in research investigating public understanding, risk perceptions, and action orientations related to climate tipping points. Given the relative novelty of the concept of climate tipping points compared with the science of anthropogenic climate change, the level of public and policymaker knowledge is likely to differ between the two. There might also be significant learning challenges associated with climate tipping points (Renn, 2022) linked to the more general challenges of understanding complex systems. This context of uneven knowledge distribution and obstacles to learning has important implications for public risk perceptions and corresponding questions of behaviour change or political engagement. In contrast with now-common studies of climate risk perceptions, existing knowledge and understanding of climate tipping points cannot be assumed.

Starting with the assumption that public knowledge of climate tipping points is likely less developed than more general knowledge of climate change, we investigate the current state of public understanding of this concept in Norway. Furthermore, we study the effects of information about climate tipping points on climate risk perceptions compared with the effects of conventional climate change communication. We base our experiment in Norway, which is an interesting case, being a major producer of oil and gas and having a fossil-based economy and a high level of education yet high levels of climate scepticism and inattention (PERITIA, 2022; YouGov, 2019). In the following, Sect. 2 briefly reviews the literature on climate risk perceptions, discussing whether and how climate tipping points present novel and specific challenges for this scholarship, and outlines Norway as a case study for this research. Section 3 outlines our methodological approach, followed by a presentation of our results (Sect. 4), a discussion (Sect. 5), and a conclusion (Sect. 6).

2 Climate tipping points: a challenge for climate risk perception research

We briefly review the vast scholarship on climate risk perceptions, focusing on the role of knowledge and highlighting insights most pertinent to tipping points (Sect. 2.1). In Sect. 2.2, we describe the characteristics of climate tipping points that might affect public risk perceptions differently than climate change more generally. This is followed by a deep dive into the still-limited literature on risk perceptions relating specifically to climate tipping points, where we identify hypotheses and existing inconclusive evidence for the effects of exposure to information about climate tipping points on public concern about climate change (Sect. 2.3). We conclude this section by bringing focus to our research location, Norway, and to why climate risk perceptions in a petroleum-based economy provide an interesting case study for our research (Sect. 2.4).

2.1 Climate change risk perceptions

The concept of climate change risk perceptions refers to individuals' subjective understandings, beliefs, and evaluations of the potential risks and impacts associated with climate change. It encompasses how people perceive the likelihood, severity, and personal relevance of impacts related to climate change. Perceptions of risk are subjective and are influenced by several factors, such as personal experience, value orientation, emotion and effect, social norms, and knowledge (Salas Reyes et al., 2021; van der Linden, 2015). Given this complexity, it is important to consider how the multiple factors interact (Capstick and Pidgeon, 2014; Kahan et al., 2012).

Climate change presents a range of risk perception challenges, especially because it operates on long time horizons, and it is perceived as a slow, incremental, and controllable phenomenon (Fox-Glassman and Weber, 2014; Sterman, 2011; Weber, 2006). Since many climate change impacts are expected to occur in the distant future, psychological distancing has played a prominent role in climate risk perception research (Jones et al., 2017; Spence et al., 2012). The psychological distance of climate change, e.g. in spatial or temporal terms, is often regarded as a barrier to climate action, although evidence for this claim has been inconsistent (Keller et al., 2022). More recent polling data and reviews suggest that the psychological distance of climate change might have been declining over the last few years; in many countries, the majority of polled citizens no longer perceive climate change as a distant threat (van Valkengoed et al., 2023). Nevertheless, climate change is associated with overall lower risk perceptions (Sterman, 2011; van Beek et al., 2022; Weber, 2006) than more abrupt and tangible phenomena, such as the COVID-19 pandemic (Manzanedo and Manning, 2020; Hochachka, 2020).

Knowledge plays a crucial role in climate change risk perceptions. Scientific knowledge (what is accepted as fact by the scientific community based on specific standards of knowledge production) can be distinguished from “public knowledge” (that which people believe to be true and act upon). Here, we are interested in the latter. However, the phenomenon we investigate occurs at the intersection of the two kinds of knowledge, where the public communication of recent scientific insights (new knowledge) is expected to create learning and belief revisions among the public.

In the context of risk perception research, van der Linden (2015) categorizes knowledge as a cognitive factor, which differs from experiential factors, socio-cultural influences, and demographics. Scientific knowledge of the risk source is the foundation for understanding climate change, for identifying and evaluating related risks, and for counteracting misconceptions (Majid et al., 2020). Some studies have shown clear correlations between instruction, information, and knowledge on the one hand and climate risk perception on the other (Aksit et al., 2018; Milfont, 2012; van der Linden, 2015; Xie et al., 2019), while others caution that there is little evidence that knowledge is a strong indicator of pro-environmental behaviour (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002) and that the relationship between knowledge and risk perceptions of climate change is more complicated.

Importantly, knowledge interacts with other variables that shape risk perceptions, especially with political belief and value systems. Adherents to different political ideologies or cultural world views experience risks related to climate change very differently (Kahan et al., 2012), driven by dynamics of motivated reasoning to protect a person's identity and core values. In Norway, the high fossil fuel dependency of the economy combined with a persistent governmental policy that the fossil fuel industry should be developed, not liquidated (Regjeringen, 2022), no doubt plays a role. Individuals with a high degree of knowledge of climate change can be found across the entire range of risk perception, from the alarmed to the dismissive (Capstick and Pidgeon, 2014; Kahan et al., 2012). Norgaard (2006, 2011) argues that it is not a lack of information that reduces risk perceptions of climate change but a psychological need to keep threatening information at a distance, informing their theory on socially organized denial.

2.2 Risk-relevant characteristics of climate tipping points

Modern science on anthropogenic climate change is over half a century old, whereas the term “climate tipping points” first emerged less than 2 decades ago in reference to Arctic ice sheet dynamics (Holland et al., 2006; Lindsay and Zhang, 2005; Winton, 2006). Since then, the use of the term “tipping point” and the corresponding body of knowledge in the climate sciences have grown rapidly (Milkoreit et al., 2018). Different definitions of climate tipping points exist, and they often identify a common set of characteristics of climate tipping processes (Armstrong McKay et al., 2022; Milkoreit et al., 2018; van Beek et al., 2022). In our study we focus on the following characteristics: multiple stable states, abruptness (non-linearity), self-amplifying (positive) feedback mechanisms, and limited reversibility (or hysteresis). Some of these characteristics, especially as non-linearity and limited reversibility, present significant aberrations from traditional conceptions of climate change as slow, incremental, and controllable. As such, climate tipping points and impacts caused by tipping processes present complex uncertainty regarding the potentially severe risks.

A state shift is the core characteristic of a tipping process, such as the potential transformation of the Amazon rainforest into a grassland (Lenton et al., 2023). State shifts imply a reorganization of the system in question, changing its main characteristics, relationships between key entities, and functions. From a human perspective, this type of change process is fundamentally different compared to incremental increases in temperature, sea-level rise, or even extreme events. System state shifts permanently remove the current environmental conditions for human life and social organization, likely forcing large-scale social reorganizations as well.

Non-linearity, i.e. self-perpetuation and acceleration of change driven by positive feedback mechanisms, is a feature of complex systems. Feedback mechanisms involve a closed loop of causality in which the change in a system is amplified (mathematically positive) or dampened/balanced (mathematically negative) (Lenton et al., 2023). Tipping points occur where positive feedback mechanisms overwhelm the balancing negative feedback mechanisms in a system, leading to self-perpetuating and amplifying the initial change, forcing a rapid transition in a non-linear manner from one stable state to another (Armstrong McKay et al., 2022; Lenton at al., 2023). Typically, humans tend to comprehend time and cause-and-effect relationships in a linear manner (Dessai and van der Sluijs, 2007) and struggle to understand non-linear changes (Pereira and Viola, 2018). While the climate system is complex, this linear model of causality has “worked”, given the well-established linear relationship between the amount of greenhouse gases (GHGs) in the Earth's atmosphere and average global temperatures, which is evident in IPCC scenarios (IPCC, 2022). However, the linear model cannot explain non-linear tipping dynamics, which present distinct learning challenges (Plate, 2010; Milkoreit et al., 2015; Renn 2022). Related to the challenges of learning about tipping risks is the observation that systemic risk perceptions are subject to attenuation and underestimation (Schweizer et al., 2022). As Schweizer et al. (2022, p. 1458) note, “they [systemic risks] are less easily understood and, due to their complexity and nonlinearity, less present in the mental representation of most people”.

Limited reversibility implies that tipping processes and the changes they create cannot be “undone” easily in the sense that the system in question will not return to its initial state even if the driver of change is removed. For example, an ice sheet might reach its tipping point and accelerate melting at a global temperature increase of 1.5 °C above pre-industrial levels. Even if global temperatures were later reduced to an increase of below 1.5 °C again, the ice sheet would not regain its mass. Reversing tipping processes is possible, but it requires different conditions (e.g. a return to much lower global temperatures in the example of the ice sheet) and, in many cases, is not achievable on timescales that are relevant for humans. Limited reversibility could have significant and undesirable psychological and emotional effects, including the weakening of agency beliefs and the creation of feelings of powerlessness, anxiety, fear, or dread (Milkoreit, 2014), leading to disengagement and avoidance (Norgaard, 2006, 2011).

In addition to these four, there are a number of additional features of tipping processes that might affect risk perceptions in a predictable way. Like climate change more generally (Enserink et al., 2013; Marx et al., 2007), knowledge about climate tipping points is subject to several types of uncertainty. Key uncertainties pertain to when (under what specific conditions) different tipping points will be reached (Sterman, 2011), how long various state-shift processes will take, and what kinds of impacts they will have over time and in which places. Recent assessments conclude that some climate tipping points can be triggered at +1–2 °C of warming (Armstrong McKay et al., 2022). Given that global average temperatures could exceed a warming of 1.5 °C as soon as the 2030s (IPCC, 2021; WMO, 2023), the risk of triggering some climate tipping points may be “dangerously close” (Lenton et al., 2019, p. 529).

While tipping processes are abrupt, they can occur over long timescales from a human perspective. These timescales differ for each tipping element, and the transition from one state to another can last from years (e.g. coral reefs) to decades (Amazon rainforest) or millennia (ice sheets), while the effects may last substantially longer (Armstrong McKay et al., 2022). Some of these timescales are short enough that human societies would struggle to adapt to the induced environmental pressures (Alley et al., 2003; Brovkin et al., 2021); however, they are of sufficient duration to invite psychological dynamics, like distancing (Spence et al., 2012) and discounting (devaluing) of future impacts (Dasgupta, 2008).

The potential impacts of climate tipping points are underexplored in the scientific literature, but knowledge about these could exert distinct effects on risk perceptions. There is general agreement that triggering climate tipping points will magnify well-established impacts of climate change (OECD, 2022), meeting the description of “dangerous climate change” (Armstrong McKay et al., 2022, p. 7). These risks include faster sea-level rise, increased intensity of extreme weather events, and abrupt ecosystem shifts (Lenton et al., 2009; OECD, 2022; Wang et al., 2023), which could significantly affect human welfare, threaten global food and water security, and destabilize societies (OECD, 2022). Furthermore, each tipping element has a certain potential to contribute to tipping cascades, which could destabilize multiple systems and ultimately have global reach (Kriegler et al., 2009; Lenton et al., 2019; Wunderling et al., 2021). These impact characteristics, especially negative-impact amplification, could lead to a more negative assessment of the future, increasing concern among the public.

2.3 Perceptions of climate tipping point risk

The literature on risk perception and communication specifically related to climate tipping points is far more limited than the voluminous body of work on climate change more broadly. Initially, scholars expected climate tipping points to have significant effects on risk perceptions among the public and policymakers, likely increasing concern. For example, Russill and Nyssa (2009) suggested that communication related to climate tipping points could encourage audiences to include non-linearity in their mental models of climate change (i.e. the potential for rapid changes) and, as a result, re-evaluate their risk perceptions. Nuttall (2012) argued that the looming threat of climate tipping points creates anticipation for the future and that this heightened attention to long-term change can aid in guiding human action. Regardless of the psychological mechanism – worries about abrupt changes or lengthened time horizons – the hypothesis that climate tipping points would increase risk perceptions created hope that they might counter mitigation inertia (Gardiner, 2009) and boost climate action.

An early study by Lowe et al. (2006) provided some evidence for this hypothesis, finding that participants were more concerned about and willing to act on climate change after watching the 2004 film The Day After Tomorrow, which depicted a fictional rapid-cooling scenario due to changes in the Atlantic Ocean current. More recently, van Beek et al. (2022) investigated changes in risk perceptions related specifically to climate tipping points using a serious game. While their quantitative analysis did not show significant effects of the intervention (possibly due to ceiling effects), they argued based on a qualitative analysis that an increase in concern and perceived seriousness of climate tipping points could be observed, even among an audience with extensive climate change knowledge and a high baseline of concern – scientists and representatives of NGOs involved in climate change negotiations.

An additional hypothesis is emerging from the recent empirical work: information about climate tipping points might have no meaningful effects on public climate risk perceptions at all. The early work by Bellamy and Hulme (2011) already indicated that higher levels of concern were limited to a distinct social group sharing a particular world view that is also associated with higher levels of concern for climate change. Then, a recent study by Formanski et al. (2022) investigated risk perceptions (and other beliefs) regarding climate tipping points with an experimental research design similar to ours. Focusing on one particular characteristic of tipping points, they studied whether participants who were given information about non-linear climate change processes would have qualitatively different risk perceptions than those presented with a common incremental change narrative. Formanski et al. (2022) found no difference between climate risk perceptions related to linear versus non-linear portrayals of climate change (based on a short message combined with a graphical depiction of future temperature change).

Each of these hypotheses – increased concern and no effects – would have different implications for public communication related to climate tipping points. Given the limited and mixed evidence for risk perception effects so far, science communication and media reporting on climate tipping points lack guidance.

Here, we seek to advance empirical understanding of this phenomenon, pursuing in particular questions about the role of knowledge as a foundation for climate risk perceptions. A number of prior studies have indicated limited public and policymaker awareness of the concept of climate tipping points. For example, Milkoreit (2019) reported limited knowledge among climate negotiators in 2018, and the survey of the UK public conducted by Bellamy (2023) showed that more than a quarter of respondents were unfamiliar with climate tipping points in 2022 despite increased media coverage of the topic. At the same time, systemic risk scholars have argued that tipping points present specific learning challenges and tend to receive less public attention than they merit (Schweizer et al., 2022; Renn, 2022). Hence, understanding the state of public knowledge, limitations in understanding, and misconceptions is important to support future communication efforts related to climate tipping points.

2.4 The Norwegian context

The focus of this study is on climate change risk perceptions in Norway, a small, oil-rich nation that perceives itself as a nation genuinely concerned about climate change (Painter, 2013; Eckersley, 2016). Norway's state-owned company Equinor is engaged in oil and gas extraction primarily for export purposes (Griffin and Heede, 2017), making Norway a significant contributor to anthropogenic GHG emissions. The Global Footprint Network (2023) reported that Norway also had one of the highest carbon footprints per capita in Europe. Contrastingly, Norway is often cited as an example of reaching a consumer tipping point in the purchase of electric vehicles, pointing to the country as a leader in decarbonizing their transport system (IEA, 2019; Sharpe and Lenton, 2021).

Recent polling data suggest that Norway is home to a significant amount of climate scepticism, with around 24 % of Norwegians not believing in anthropogenic climate change (Krange et al., 2019; YouGov, 2019). At the same time, Norway is facing visible signs of climate change, with increased rainfall and frequency of landslides along the west coast (Hanssen-Bauer et al., 2015). However, research conducted by KANTAR (2020) found that only a third of the population in Norway noticed the ongoing consequences of climate change around them. Along with current and future effects of climate change, Norway is likely to be physically affected by the impacts of a number of identified climate tipping points, such as thawing of mountain glaciers and permafrost, shifting boreal forests, melting ice sheets, and ocean circulation destabilization. The rapid decline of glaciers is likely not perceived as a real risk, and the same holds for the vanishing permafrost in northern parts of the county (e.g. Finnmark) or on the island of Svalbard, since it does not pose critical risk to human infrastructure.

Public knowledge of climate tipping points would have to be based on the consumption of media reporting. As Bellamy (2023) shows, media coverage of climate tipping points has significantly increased in international English-language reporting over the last 20 years, especially since 2018. To understand whether and to what extent Norwegian newspapers have been covering the topic of climate tipping points relative to general climate change, we conducted a quantitative analysis of Norwegian media using the database available through the National Library of Norway. Our search covers the time period from 2005 to 2022 and over 100 Norwegian press newspapers (local and national) for articles containing the following terms in Norwegian: global warming, climate change, climate crisis, tipping point, and climate + tipping point (global oppvarming, klimaendring, klimakrise, vippepunkt, and klima+vippepunkt). As expected, we found substantially more media content on climate-related terms without mentions of tipping points. The first article mentioning climate tipping points was published in April 2006 in the newspaper Klassekampen and focused on irreversible climate changes. It was entitled “Is it too late to turn back?”. Coverage of the subject remained limited (less than 50 articles yr−1) until 2017 and has been expanding since 2018, mirroring Bellamy's analysis of British and international news media.

https://esd.copernicus.org/articles/15/969/2024/esd-15-969-2024-f01

Figure 1Norwegian mentions in press newspaper articles of climate change and climate tipping points.

Download

Mentions of climate tipping points likely became more common after the publication of the Norwegian popular science book The World on a Tipping Point (Verden på vippepunktet) by Hessen (2020), which received wide public attention. Based on these findings, we expect knowledge of climate tipping points in the general population to be lower than that of general climate change.

3 Research design and methods

3.1 Survey design

Our study sought to answer the following research questions:

  1. What is the level of knowledge of climate tipping points among Norwegians?

  2. To what extent does the information on climate tipping points increase concern about climate change?

To answer both questions, we conducted a web-based survey with an embedded experiment, which was implemented by a third-party polling service in Norway. The survey consisted of three parts. In part 1a, all participants were asked a series of questions about their climate change risk perceptions, including concern, seriousness of climate change, and the need to act (see q1r1, q1r2, and q1r3 in Table S1) using a five-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 “completely agree” to 5 “completely disagree”, providing a moderate level of granularity for respondents to express their opinions. The internal consistency reliability of the questions measuring climate change risk perceptions (CCRPs) was assessed using Cronbach's alpha. The calculated value obtained for Cronbach's alpha was α=0.897, indicating strong internal consistency reliability among the questions measuring CCRPs, suggesting that the questions are highly correlated and likely measure the same underlying construct effectively. Part 1b assessed knowledge about climate tipping points (research question 1). Our survey design sought to gauge general familiarity with the concept without providing or asking for a definition or description of characteristics of the phenomenon. The latter is not necessarily a reliable indicator of public understanding of a difficult scientific concept, where multiple definitions exist within and across disciplines (Milkoreit et al., 2018) and continue to be debated. Relying on free recall and knowledge self-assessment, we used a Likert scale to assess perceived familiarity with the concept of climate tipping points (vippepunkter). The scale items ranged from “never heard of it” to “know it well”, including “I am not sure/I do not know”. If the participant indicated at least some familiarity with climate tipping points (“know it well”, “a little familiar”, “neutral”), they were asked if they could give an example (yes/no question). Those who answered yes were prompted to provide a written example to demonstrate actual knowledge (see q3b in Table S1). This approach was grounded in cognitive theories of concepts and learning (prototype and exemplar theories), which rely on category formation based on examples of the phenomenon in question (e.g. Hampton, 2006; Park, 2013). We assumed that providing an example was an easier cognitive task than providing a definition. This design, presenting increasingly challenging questions that combine self-evaluation, recall, and a knowledge “check”, allowed participants to reveal their degree of familiarity with the concept.

This design contrasts with that of Bellamy (2023), who presented survey participants with a definition and 10 examples of climate tipping points identified in the literature and asked for self-reported familiarity with these. We purposefully did not present participants with a definition or examples but sought to elicit information about their knowledge based on their ability to recall examples themselves. This limits the influence of biases like socially desirable responding (e.g. projecting knowledgeability).

For part 2 of the survey, participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions, receiving different kinds of climate change information in text form. The participants in Group 1 were asked to read a text that introduced them to the concept of climate tipping points and included specific characteristics of tipping points identified in the literature (non-linear and abrupt change, irreversibility of climate change, system interactions, and domino effects). The text for Group 2 presented more general information about climate change without terminology pertaining to climate tipping points. The texts were comparable in length (each took 2–3 min to read) and with the same intended linguistic style and difficulty. They were significantly longer than the texts used by Formanski et al. (2022) but still short relative to a common news article. The texts were presented in Norwegian (English translations can be found in the Supplement).

In part 3, all participants were asked the same questions presented in part 1a about their risk perceptions related to climate change (see q5r1, q5r2, and q5r3 in Table S1).

The responses to the survey were managed using SPSS data files and were later converted into Microsoft Excel spreadsheets for processing and analysis in R. Our results were verified by an unbiased and impartial third party.

The survey design is visualized in Fig. 2.

https://esd.copernicus.org/articles/15/969/2024/esd-15-969-2024-f02

Figure 2Survey design outline.

Download

Our survey was conducted by an external Norwegian data collection unit (Opinion) in October–November 2022. A quota sample of participants was recruited from a pool of over 8000. Our sample included 851 adults ranging from 18–91 years of age with a 50/50 split between men and women from all regions of Norway (northern Norway, 9 %; central Norway, 14 %; western Norway, 20 %; eastern Norway, 30 %; southern Norway, 14 %; Oslo, 13 %) and did not favour any specific characteristics (pro-environmental views, political orientation, level of education, etc.). It is important to note that we attained a “quota” sample and not necessarily a “nationally representative” sample of the Norwegian population which is limited mainly to gender, age, and geographic location. This leaves space for some members of the population to be excluded, such as immigrants, international students, refugees, people with disabilities, and non-binary participants. This limits our study in reflecting certain dimensions of diversity and should be considered in future recruitment processes. While our study provides valuable insights within the scope of our sample, we acknowledge the need for caution when generalizing the findings beyond our specific sample group.

3.2 Analysis

We used a primarily qualitative approach to assess knowledge and a statistical analysis to analyse changes in risk perceptions.

  • Knowledge

    Using data from pre-intervention questions about knowledge and adjusting the approach of Milkoreit (2019), we categorized participants into four different levels of knowledge (no knowledge, incorrect knowledge, some knowledge, good knowledge). Participants who indicated that they were not familiar with the concept of climate tipping points at all or that they had “little knowledge” were categorized as having “no knowledge” (category 1). All remaining participants were regarded as having some knowledge based on their self-assessment. Those who indicated that they could not provide an example were categorized as “some knowledge, but not demonstrated” (category 2a). Similarly, if a participant answered yes (indicating a self-assessed ability to offer an example) but then did not provide a response to the prompt for an example, they were also categorized as “some knowledge, but not demonstrated” (category 2a). It is possible that some of these participants had some form of knowledge about climate tipping points and their characteristics but did not provide written examples for reasons other than an inability to recall this information, e.g. time constraints or a general unwillingness to answer open-ended questions. It is also possible that they would have been able to demonstrate knowledge if we had specifically asked for a definition, a description, or other associated ideas. However, we assumed that the most likely reason for not providing any text was the inability to recall and provide relevant information.

    Participants who responded to the prompt to provide an example of a climate tipping point were categorized based on the content of their answer. The answers contained both correct and incorrect examples of tipping elements, such as “Arctic sea ice” or “Gulf Stream”, and more general (unelicited) descriptions of the concept, such as “point of no return” or “an irreversible event”. These open-ended answers were coded to distinguish between “some but incorrect knowledge” (category 2b) and “good knowledge” (category 3) (see Table S2). Individuals who had provided either a false example or a description that did not contain any tipping point characteristics identified in the literature were placed in category 2b; those who had identified at least one correct example of tipping points or correctly described one or more characteristics of tipping points were placed in category 3.

    • 1.

      No knowledge: self-reported lack or limitation of familiarity with climate tipping points.

    • 2a.

      Some knowledge but not demonstrated: self-reported familiarity with climate tipping points but answered no to the question about the ability to provide an example or answered yes to the question about the ability to provide an example but did not respond to the prompt to provide an example.

    • 2b.

      Some knowledge but incorrect: self-reported familiarity and ability to provide an example but inability to provide a correct example of a tipping point or any description (feature) that could be associated with climate tipping points.

    • 3.

      Good knowledge: identified one or more correct examples or at least one correct feature of climate tipping processes.

    These categories are based on rather moderate expectations of what constitutes knowledge and maximize assumptions in favour of knowledge. This approach likely overestimates the state of actively usable knowledge that could shape risk perceptions. Category 2 especially likely includes participants with a very limited understanding of the phenomenon.

    While none of our survey questions asked participants to identify characteristics of climate tipping points, we found it meaningful to code these open responses in addition to specific examples of climate tipping points as instances of demonstrated knowledge. We also coded these unexpected responses for common characteristics of climate tipping points – alternative stable states, threshold, positive feedback, non-linearity, limited reversibility (Milkoreit et al., 2017) – and analysed how frequently each of these were mentioned (see Table S3).

    We assessed public familiarity with specific tipping processes by counting how often they were mentioned as examples by participants. Here it was necessary to distinguish types/classes of tipping elements and specific examples within each type. The different types included cryosphere tipping elements/ice sheets, circulation patterns in the oceans and atmosphere, and biosphere tipping elements. Some participants referred to these types of tipping elements, while others provided more specific examples, such as the West Antarctic Ice Sheet or the Amazon rainforest. Based on these counts, we assessed which known tipping elements the public is currently most familiar with.

  • Risk perceptions

    Our survey data were quantitatively analysed using data analysis tools in Microsoft Excel and R in order to identify any effect on climate risk perceptions post-intervention between the two groups – analysis for covariance (ANCOVA analysis) and t-test (two samples assuming unequal variances) – and within the same group (t-test: paired two samples for means). Significance tests were performed on the data in order to identify any statistically significant differences in responses on concern levels for climate change post-intervention.

4 Results

4.1 General state of knowledge of climate tipping points

When asked about their self-reported level of familiarity with climate tipping points, 6 % (n=53) indicated good levels of familiarity, 22 % (n=183) indicated some familiarity, 15 % (n=126) were neutral, 23 % (n=196) indicated very little knowledge, 29 % (n=244) indicated they had never heard of the term, and 5 % (n=49) answered they were unsure or did not know. Based on our categorization, 52 % (n=440) had no knowledge about climate tipping points. About 42 % (n=362) of the participants indicated some knowledge about climate tipping points and were asked whether they could give an example. More than half of these (n=201) answered no. Among those who answered yes to this question, eight did not provide a response. Hence, 25 % of participants (n=209) fall into category 2a – some but no demonstrated knowledge. This left us with 153 responses to the open-ended question. When investigating the demographics of this group of respondents, we found that the age group under 30 was under-represented (12.4 % or n=19), while the age group 60+ was overrepresented (35 % or n=54). Additionally, we found that respondents identifying as men were over-represented (64.7 % or n=99), while respondents identifying as women were under-represented (35.3 % or n=54). The small number of participants (5 % or n=44) who provided incorrect responses, which included false examples or descriptions of climate change generally, such as “global warming” or “increasing average global temperatures”, fall into category 2b – incorrect knowledge. Finally, 13 % (n=109) demonstrated good knowledge, i.e. were able to provide a correct example or identify at least one correct feature of tipping processes (see Fig. 3).

https://esd.copernicus.org/articles/15/969/2024/esd-15-969-2024-f03

Figure 3Results on the level of knowledge of climate tipping points amongst participants.

Download

Given the self-reported lack of familiarity among 52 % of the population (category 1) and the lack of ability to recall (correct) information about climate tipping points among another 30 % of participants (categories 2a and 2b), we argue that four out of five Norwegians lack knowledge of the concept in the sense that their understanding is insufficient to meaningfully inform a person's risk perceptions related to climate change (see the Supplement for more details).

After the intervention, participants were asked whether the text they had read contained information that was new to them (see q4r1 in Table S1). A higher percentage (27 %) of participants who read the text on climate tipping points agreed that the information was new to them compared with the general climate change group (17 %). This difference was highly statistically significant (t (848) =5,98, p<0.05). However, this result does not align with the result of participants indicating a lack of familiarity with the concept of climate tipping points in part 1b of the survey (more than 50 %). This disparity between the initial self-report of knowledge and the post-experimental assessment of the information's novelty indicates reliability problems regarding self-reported data, possibly linked to a desire for socially desirable responses. This difference could also be explained by the fact that the free recall of memorized information is a more challenging cognitive task than the recognition of previously encountered information. In other words, participants might not have been able to recall the definition or examples of climate tipping points in part 1b of the survey but later remembered having heard or read about the concept when they encountered the materials provided for the experiment.

4.2 Characteristics of climate tipping points

Our survey did not include an explicit question about the characteristics of climate tipping points, so we cannot draw any conclusions from our analysis about familiarity with characteristics in the Norwegian population. However, many of the participants who were presented with and chose to respond to the prompt to provide an example of a tipping process (n=153 or 18 %) responded by providing descriptive comments instead of or in addition to an example. These comments identified characteristics of climate tipping points and provided an unexpected opportunity to add a layer of analysis about current knowledge patterns within the more knowledgeable population segment. We calculated the frequency with which specific characteristics of tipping points were mentioned by participants in this sub-group of 153 participants to identify the most and least common features in public understanding (see also Table S7.).

Participants mentioned limited reversibility most frequently (n=46 or 30 % among this sub-group), with some using the term “irreversible” directly or phrases such as “unable to turn back” or “point of no return”. Feedback dynamics were the second most common characteristic mentioned (n=18 or 12 %), with phrases such as “self-reinforcing loops” or, more frequently, detailed descriptions of feedback loops, such as “less ice allows more light absorption which leads to more ice melting”. A total of 13 participants (8 %) used terms including a “threshold”, “boundary”, or “limit” that is crossed to refer to critical thresholds. Very few individuals mentioned non-linearity (n=8) or multiple stable states (n=4). For abruptness and non-linearity, participants used terms such as “escalating” to describe change or stated that climate change will happen “even faster.” The idea of multiple stable states was described with the terms “unstable”, “fluctuating”, or “change from one system to another”. Other features such as severe impacts and uncertainty were not mentioned often enough to be regarded as part of a common understanding.

4.3 Examples of climate tipping points

By far the most frequently identified type of tipping points were those related to the cryosphere – 71 mentions (provided by 61 participants, some mentioning multiple elements) referred to ice loss, especially the Greenland Ice Sheet and the Arctic sea ice. The majority of these referred to “ice melting” or “polar ice” and “glaciers disappearing” without specific geographical reference. Some participants referred to “glaciers” but did not specify if these were mountain glaciers specifically; therefore these responses were coded as ice loss generally. More specific examples included “permafrost”, the Greenland Ice Sheet, and the loss of sea ice in the Arctic.

The second most frequent type of tipping element was circulation patterns (seven mentions) followed by biosphere components (four mentions). Mentions of circulation patterns included mentions of “the Gulf Stream” or “ocean currents”, and there was one mention of “air currents”. Regarding biosphere components, only one person identified the “coral reefs” and two people identified the “Amazon rainforest”. The results are summarized in Fig. 4 (data used for this figure can be found in Table S4.).

https://esd.copernicus.org/articles/15/969/2024/esd-15-969-2024-f04

Figure 4Most commonly and correctly identified climate tipping elements by participants.

Download

4.4 Effect of information about climate tipping points on level of concern for climate change

All participants were asked about their concerns about climate change before and after our intervention (survey questions q1r1 and q5r2 found in Table S1). A series of statistical analyses were performed in R. Firstly, an ANOVA was performed, comparing the post-intervention scores to the pre-intervention scores, which shows a statistical significance (F (1, 848) = 1962.1, p<0.000). A second ANOVA test found there was also a statistical significance when comparing Groups 1 and 2 post-intervention (F (1, 848) = 3.998, p<0.05 [= 0.046]) but not when comparing Groups 1 and 2 pre-intervention (F(1, 848) = 0.892, p= 0.345). This indicates that our experimental treatment (reading a brief text with information about climate tipping points) significantly contributed to a change in risk perceptions of climate change in Group 1, while it (text on climate change) did not have an effect on Group 2. An ANCOVA combining pre-intervention and post-intervention comparisons, as well as group comparisons, resulted in statistically significant differences in both treatment differences (pre/post) (t=44.301, p<0.000) and group differences (t=-2.201, p=0.028). For data used in our ANCOVA analysis, including mean values and standard deviations for each experimental condition, see Table 1.

Table 1Results of statistical analysis.

Download Print Version | Download XLSX

From our analysis, the effect size of the post-intervention and pre-intervention score difference without any adjustment for pre-scores is Cohen's d 0.14, which would be regarded as very small according to Cohen (1988) with a confidence interval of 0.00–0.27. Furthermore, we find the effect size difference between Group 1 and Group 2 in the standardized post-intervention score to be 0.08 standard deviation units, based on Field (2014). The interpretation of our data finds that, while there is a statistical difference between the two groups post-intervention, the effect is very small.

The difference in responses post-intervention for the two groups is illustrated in Fig. 6. The biggest change in responses before and after our intervention was that some who agreed before the intervention that they were concerned about climate change completely agreed that they are personally concerned after the intervention. Both Groups 1 and 2 saw shifts of this nature; however, Group 1, who were presented with information on climate tipping points, saw a higher degree of difference post-intervention.

https://esd.copernicus.org/articles/15/969/2024/esd-15-969-2024-f05

Figure 5Percentage difference in climate risk perceptions post-intervention per group (Group 1: climate tipping points text; Group 2: climate change general text).

Download

A paired t-test for means was performed on Group 1 between their level of concern before and after the intervention in Microsoft Excel. The results indicate that the responses from Group 1 were significantly different post-intervention (t (419) = 2.72, p<0.05). The same test was carried out for Group 2, and it was found that the difference in response post-intervention was not statistically significant (t (430) =0.07, p>0.05).

5 Discussion

Despite our expectations that knowledge of climate tipping points would be more limited than knowledge of climate change in general, we were surprised to find that more than 80 % of respondents were not sufficiently familiar with the concept to provide an example. Ultimately, only 13 % of Norwegians have an understanding of climate tipping points that might be sufficient to serve as a foundation for risk assessments and potential behavioural changes. Even within this more knowledgeable segment of the population, understanding of climate tipping points and familiarity with examples are heavily skewed towards a specific type of tipping process (ice loss). These findings indicate a potential knowledge gap between scientists and the general public, with important implications for understanding risk perceptions, policy support, or behavioural change related to climate change. For the large majority of Norwegians, knowledge of climate tipping points likely does not yet affect judgements of climate risk.

Comparing our assessment to that of Bellamy (2023), the state of public knowledge in Norway appears to be significantly weaker than that in the UK. Bellamy reported that 25 % of British study participants had not heard of any of the 10 explicitly named climate tipping points before taking their survey and that awareness of the issue is still low in the UK. These observations could be indicative of more limited media communication on climate tipping points in Norway compared to British and international press, but they could also be the result of different methodological approaches. Our research design relied on participants' free recall of examples, while Bellamy provided survey participants with a list of 10 tipping points and asked whether they had heard of these before. The latter is a less-demanding cognitive task than open recall, which might account for some of the differences and suggest that familiarity in Norway might be higher than our findings reflect.

At the same time, Bellamy's findings strongly mirror our own regarding the awareness of specific tipping elements. In both countries, there is significant variation, with the highest scores for tipping elements in the cryosphere (e.g. over 50 % of British participants were familiar with Arctic sea ice loss) and the potential dieback of the Amazon rainforest and with the lowest scores (under 20 % in the UK) for the risk of AMOC collapse and boreal forest dieback. Cryosphere elements are the most commonly mentioned examples of climate tipping points in our study. This may be due to the fact that Arctic sea ice was the first Earth system component to be associated with tipping points (Winton, 2006) and likely also due to its significance to Norway, which is proximal to the Arctic. The prominence of ice-related examples may also be due to the rather simple cause-and-effect relationship between higher temperatures and melting ice sheets and to the prevalence of cryosphere change in visual media reporting, e.g. eye-catching photos of polar bears on (disappearing) icebergs. The reasons for the differential popularity of, recognizability of, and attention to various tipping elements should be explored in future research. While Arctic summer sea ice is no longer regarded as having a tipping point (Armstrong McKay et al., 2022), other elements of the cryosphere (e.g. Greenland Ice Sheet, West Antarctic Ice Sheet) remain policy-relevant, with significant impacts on human systems. More surprising is the lack of public awareness of the AMOC as a potential tipping point, with potentially dramatic consequences for Norway and all Atlantic states. Recent studies find that the circulation system is at its weakest in 1600 years (Boers, 2021; Thornalley et al., 2018), and some have argued that it could cross a tipping point this century (Ditlevsen and Ditlevsen, 2023), yet this is a highly disputed worst-case scenario.

More generally, the shared finding that there is low public awareness of climate tipping points in these two countries indicates the learning challenges related to tipping processes as complex system dynamics that defy mechanistic causal thinking. Media reporting on climate tipping points has been increasing over the last 5 years but with limited effects on public understanding so far. Given this baseline of limited knowledge paired with cognitive and emotional barriers to learning, it is likely that our experimental intervention (a short, fact-based description of climate tipping points) had very small effects on risk perceptions because of its limited potential to contribute to learning and understanding.

Our analysis provides modest evidence for the hypothesis that climate tipping point communication can increase public concern about climate change compared to more conventional, linear descriptions of climate change (Russill, 2015). We observed that the strongest change in risk perceptions occurs among those who already are concerned about climate change, which aligns with findings by van Beek et al. (2022), although our survey-embedded experiment was significantly less engaging than the serious game deployed in their study. Our results differ from recent findings by Formanski et al. (2022), who found no difference in risk perceptions between participants presented with portrayals of linear versus non-linear climate change. This difference may simply be due to our larger sample size (n=851 versus n=360), as small effects may become more significant when the sample size is larger. Formanski et al. (2022) found that there may be no effect of non-linear portrayals of climate change on public risk perceptions, but if there is an effect it is likely to be very small. Our research finds that an effect may indeed exist, and it was statistically significant in our sample, but the effect was indeed very small. We cannot claim that our results contrast with previous studies but only that they differ and may indicate that an effect may exist. One explanation for this difference might be that Formanski et al. (2022) focused on a specific characteristic of tipping points (non-linearity), which might not be the feature that generates most concern. Although we did not explicitly ask participants about their familiarity with different characteristics of tipping processes and cannot draw any general conclusions from our data, limited reversibility was the most commonly identified feature of climate tipping points in the open responses provided by a subset of our study participants. This is similar to findings by Milkoreit (2019) from surveys with international policymakers, who also indicated the most concern with irreversibility. It could be argued that people are more concerned about the permanence of losses rather than the speed of change, especially when limited reversibility is combined with the possibility of severe harm (“catastrophic risks”). While we cannot conclude this from our study, it opens up pathways for future research.

Major questions remain regarding how to best communicate the risks of climate change, balancing information about threats with motivation to act and managing a complicated medley of emotions, including fear and apathy. This discussion is particularly relevant for Norway with the paradoxical gap between the political acceptance of climate risks and continued oil exploration and the apparent widespread ignorance or denial of climate change (PERITIA, 2022; YouGov, 2019). While communicating risk-based scientific predictions of potential tipping points should motivate climate action among both politicians and voters, the concept is hardly known and partially misunderstood. One could argue that, in Norway, a general feeling of safety and trust in government and technological solutions is widespread, creating a kind of hubris with regard to climate risks that can only be overcome by personal experience as the situation worsens (see Lujala et al., 2015). However, there is major potential to increase the scale and effectiveness of public communication about the risks of climate tipping points. Future research should seek to support the development of effective communication strategies by considering national differences, including differences in cultural world views (Bellamy, 2023).

6 Conclusions

Understanding climate change risk perceptions is crucial for effective communication, policymaking, and public engagement. Climate tipping points, while presenting a range of threats to societies, might also provide new communication tools and opportunities to reshape existing climate change narratives, public risk perceptions, engagement, and support for climate action.

Our study investigated the level of knowledge of climate tipping points among participants in Norway and assessed whether information pertaining to climate tipping points has a different impact on climate change risk perceptions compared with information about climate change more generally. Among our findings, two stand out. Firstly, we found a widespread lack of knowledge about climate tipping points among Norwegians, suggesting that the topic remains “new” for the majority of the population despite its increasing presence in the media. This situation merits further investigation, focusing on the question of how to best support public learning and meaning-making related to tipping points, including active learning strategies (van Beek et al., 2022; Formanski et al., 2022) in the face of significant obstacles to informal learning.

Secondly, our results indicate a very small effect of information about climate tipping points on risk perceptions of Norwegians, while more general climate change information had no significant effect. We suspect that these limited effects are linked to the general state of public knowledge in Norway and the limited effects of our intervention on participants' understanding of the concept and its potential implications for human wellbeing. In other words, the reading materials provided to participants did not (in most cases) enable learning about tipping points in a way that affected existing risk perceptions. Our experimental treatment might have been ineffective as a learning device. Given the findings and limitations of our study, we recommend further investigation into how laypeople and decision-makers learn about the risks posed by climate tipping processes and how (or if) knowledge about tipping points changes existing perceptions of climate change risk. Future work should explore in particular whether different modes of communication, engagement, and learning have different impacts, e.g. active learning strategies such as serious gaming or passive learning through reading news or story-based information. This work might require more challenging experimental designs (e.g. game or storytelling workshops) coupled with in-depth interviews, focus groups, or observations.

Given that climate risk perceptions are shaped by multiple factors in addition to knowledge (Kahan et al., 2012; Libarkin et al., 2018), future research should also explore how (changes in) knowledge and understanding interact with other variables over time to investigate the complex psychological processes that may be triggered by forewarnings of climate tipping points. Future work should not only consider the role of cultural cognition in the adoption of this concept but also emotional and social barriers to learning, such as psychological distancing, identity protection, and socially organized denial (Norgard, 2011). Future research should also explore the relationship between climate risk perceptions and the action gap.

Code and data availability

Survey dataset and statistical code used for producing the results have been archived within the open-access repository on Zenodo (Nadeau, 2024).

Supplement

The supplement related to this article is available online at: https://doi.org/10.5194/esd-15-969-2024-supplement.

Author contributions

All authors contributed to the conceptualisation of the study and developed the methodology. CN was responsible for the formal analysis. CN, MM, and DOH contributed to the original draft. THE provided feedback and comments. CN and MM revised and edited the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The contact author has declared that none of the authors has any competing interests.

Disclaimer

Publisher’s note: Copernicus Publications remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims made in the text, published maps, institutional affiliations, or any other geographical representation in this paper. While Copernicus Publications makes every effort to include appropriate place names, the final responsibility lies with the authors.

Special issue statement

This article is part of the special issue “Tipping points in the Anthropocene”. It is a result of the “Tipping Points: From Climate Crisis to Positive Transformation” international conference hosted by the Global Systems Institute (GSI) and University of Exeter (12–14 September 2022), as well as the associated creation of a Tipping Points Research Alliance by GSI and the Potsdam Institute for Climate Research, Exeter, Great Britain, 12–14 September 2022.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Opinion for their collaboration in this research and their role in conducting a public opinion poll in Norway.

Financial support

This work was supported by the UiO: Norden initiative at the University of Oslo and the Centre of Biogeochemistry in the Anthropocene (CBA).

Review statement

This paper was edited by Caroline Zimm and reviewed by three anonymous referees.

References

Aksit, O., McNeal, K. S., Gold, A. U., Libarkin, J. C., and Harris, S.: The influence of instruction, prior knowledge, and values on climate change risk perception among undergraduates, J. Res. Sci. Teach., 55, 550–572, https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21430, 2018. 

Alley, R. B., Marotzke, J., Nordhaus, W. D., Overpeck, J. T., Peteet, D. M., Jr, R. A. P., Pierrehumbert, R. T., Rhines, P. B., Stocker, T. F., Talley, L. D., and Wallace, J. M.: Abrupt Climate Change, Science, 299, 2005–2010, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1081056, 2003. 

Armstrong McKay, D. I., Staal, A., Abrams, J. F., Winkelmann, R., Sakschewski, B., Loriani, S., Fetzer, I., Cornell, S. E., Rockström, J., and Lenton, T. M.: Exceeding 1.5 °C global warming could trigger multiple climate tipping points, Science, 377, eabn7950, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abn7950, 2022. 

Bellamy, R.: Public perceptions of climate tipping points, Public Underst. Sci., 32, 1033–1047, https://doi.org/10.1177/09636625231177820, 2023. 

Bellamy, R. and Hulme, M.: Beyond the Tipping Point: Understanding Perceptions of Abrupt Climate Change and Their Implications, Weather Clim. Soc., 3, 48–60, https://doi.org/10.1175/2011WCAS1081.1, 2011. 

Bergquist, M., Nilsson, A., Harring, N., and Jagers, S. C.: Meta-analyses of fifteen determinants of public opinion about climate change taxes and laws, Nat. Clim. Change, 12, 235–240, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-022-01297-6, 2022. 

Boers, N.: Observation-based early-warning signals for a collapse of the Atlantic Meridional Overturning Circulation, Nat. Clim. Change, 11, 680–688, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-021-01097-4, 2021. 

Brovkin, V., Brook, E., Williams, J. W., Bathiany, S., Lenton, T. M., Barton, M., DeConto, R. M., Donges, J. F., Ganopolski, A., McManus, J., Praetorius, S., De Vernal, A., Abe-Ouchi, A., Cheng, H., Claussen, M., Crucifix, M., Gallopín, G., Iglesias, V., Kaufman, D. S., Kleinen, T., Lambert, F., Van Der Leeuw, S., Liddy, H., Loutre, M.-F., McGee, D., Rehfeld, K., Rhodes, R., Seddon, A. W. R., Trauth, M. H., Vanderveken, L., and Yu, Z.: Past abrupt changes, tipping points and cascading impacts in the Earth system, Nat. Geosci., 14, 550–558, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41561-021-00790-5, 2021. 

Capstick, S. B. and Pidgeon, N. F.: What is climate change scepticism? Examination of the concept using a mixed methods study of the UK public, Glob. Environ. Change, 24, 389–401, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.08.012, 2014. 

Cohen, J.: Set Correlation and Contingency Tables, Appl. Psychol. Meas., 12, 425–434, https://doi.org/10.1177/014662168801200410, 1988. 

Dasgupta, P.: Discounting climate change, J. Risk Uncertainty, 37, 141–169, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-008-9049-6, 2008. 

Dessai, S. and Sluijs, J. van der: Uncertainty and climate change adaptation: a scoping study, Copernicus Institute for Sustainable Development and Innovation, Department of Science Technology and Society, Utrecht, ISBN: 978-90-8672-025-5, 2007. 

Ditlevsen, P. and Ditlevsen, S.: Warning of a forthcoming collapse of the Atlantic meridional overturning circulation, Nat. Commun., 14, 4254, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-39810-w, 2023. 

Drews, S. and Van Den Bergh, J. C. J. M.: What explains public support for climate policies? A review of empirical and experimental studies, Clim. Pol., 16, 855–876, https://doi.org/10.1080/14693062.2015.1058240, 2016. 

Eckersley, R.: National identities, international roles, and the legitimation of climate leadership: Germany and Norway compared, Environ. Polit., 25, 180–201, https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2015.1076278, 2016. 

Enserink, B., Kwakkel, J. H., and Veenman, S.: Coping with uncertainty in climate policy making: (Mis)understanding scenario studies, Futures, 53, 1–12, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.futures.2013.09.006, 2013. 

Field, A.: Discovering statistics using SPSS: and sex and drugs and rock'n'roll, 3rd Edn., Sage, Los Angeles, 821 pp., https://doi.org/10.1024/1012-5302/a000397, 2014. 

Formanski, F. J., Pein, M. M., Loschelder, D. D., Engler, J.-O., Husen, O., and Majer, J. M.: Tipping points ahead? How laypeople respond to linear versus nonlinear climate change predictions, Climatic Change, 175, 8, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-022-03459-z, 2022. 

Fox-Glassman, K. and Weber, E.: Natural Hazards and Climate Change as Dread Risk, 573552014-102, https://doi.org/10.1037/e573552014-102, 2014. 

Gardiner, S. M.: Saved by Disaster? Abrupt Climate Change, Political Inertia, and the Possibility of an Intergenerational Arms Race, J. Soc. Philos., 40, 140–162, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9833.2009.01444.x, 2009. 

Global Footprint Network: Country overshoot days 2022, Earth Overshoot Day, https://www.overshootday.org/content/uploads/2022/01/Country_Overshoot_Days_2022.pdf, last access: 12 March 2023. 

Griffin, D. P. and Heede, C. R.: The Carbon Majors Database CDP Carbon Majors Report 2017, Climate Accountability Institute, USA, CID: 20.500.12592/1d4m38, https://policycommons.net/artifacts/3085142/untitled/3885955/ (last access: 2 July 2023), 2017. 

Hampton, J. A.: Concepts as Prototypes, in: Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 46, Elsevier, 79–113, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(06)46003-5, 2006. 

Hanssen-Bauer, I., Førland, E.J., Haddeland, I., Hisdal, H., Lawrence, D., Mayer, S., Nesje, A., Nilsen, J., Sandven, S., Sandø, A., and Sorteberg, A.: Climate in Norway 2100 – A knowledge base for climate adaptation, NCCS report no. 1/2017, Norwegian Environment Agency (Miljødirektoratet), https://www.miljodirektoratet.no/globalassets/publikasjoner/M741/M741.pdf (last access: 4 March 2023), 2017. 

Hessen, D. O.: Verden På Vippepunktet: Hvor ille kan det bli?, Res publica, Oslo, ISBN: 9788282262019, 2020. 

Hochachka, G.: Unearthing insights for climate change response in the midst of the COVID-19 pandemic, Glob. Sustain., 3, e33, https://doi.org/10.1017/sus.2020.27, 2020. 

Holland, M. M., Bitz, C. M., and Tremblay, B.: Future abrupt reductions in the summer Arctic sea ice, Geophys. Res. Lett., 33, L23503, https://doi.org/10.1029/2006GL028024, 2006. 

IEA (International Energy Agency): Renewables 2022 – Analysis and forecast to 2027, https://iea.blob.core.windows.net/assets/ada7af90-e280-46c4-a577-df2e4fb44254/Renewables2022.pdf (last access: 13 May 2023), 2022. 

IPCC: Intergovernmental Panel On Climate Change: Climate Change 2021 – The Physical Science Basis: Working Group I Contribution to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, 1st Edn., Cambridge University Press, https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009157896, 2023. 

Jones, C., Hine, D. W., and Marks, A. D.: The future is now: Reducing psychological distance to increase public engagement with climate change, Risk Anal., 37, 331–341, https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12601, 2017. 

Kahan, D. M., Peters, E., Wittlin, M., Slovic, P., Ouellette, L. L., Braman, D., and Mandel, G.: The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks, Nat. Clim. Change, 2, 732–735, https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1547, 2012. 

KANTAR: Gallup Kantar Klimabarometer 2020, https://kantar.no/globalassets/fra-webnodes/ (last access: 3 March 2023), 2020. 

Keller, E., Marsh, J. E., Richardson, B. H., and Ball, L. J.: A systematic review of the psychological distance of climate change: Towards the development of an evidence-based construct, J. Environ. Psychol., 81, 101822, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101822, 2022. 

Kollmuss, A. and Agyeman, J.: Mind the Gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior?, Environ. Educ. Res., 8, 239–260, https://doi.org/10.1080/13504620220145401, 2002. 

Krange, O., Kaltenborn, B. P., and Hultman, M.: Cool dudes in Norway: climate change denial among conservative Norwegian men, Environ. Soc., 5, 1–11, https://doi.org/10.1080/23251042.2018.1488516, 2019. 

Kriegler, E., Hall, J. W., Held, H., Dawson, R., and Schellnhuber, H. J.: Imprecise probability assessment of tipping points in the climate system, P. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 106, 5041–5046, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0809117106, 2009. 

Kuramochi, T., den Elzen, M., and Fransen, T.: Chap. 3, Nationally determined contributions and long-term pledges: The global landscape and G20 member progress - Emissions Gap Report 2022: The Closing Window – Climate Crisis Calls for Rapid Transformation of Societies, 11-25, https://wedocs.unep.org/20.500.11822/40927 (last access: 26 April 2023), 2022. 

Lenton, T. M., Held, H., Kriegler, E., Hall, J. W., Lucht, W., Rahmstorf, S., and Schellnhuber, H. J.: Tipping elements in the Earth's climate system, P. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 105, 1786–1793, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0705414105, 2008. 

Lenton, T. M., Rockström, J., Gaffney, O., Rahmstorf, S., Richardson, K., Steffen, W., and Schellnhuber, H. J.: Climate tipping points – too risky to bet against, Nature, 575, 592–595, https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-019-03595-0, 2019. 

Lenton, T. M., Armstrong McKay, D. I., Loriani, S., Abrams, J. F., Lade, S. J., Donges, J. F., Milkoreit, M., Powell, T., Smith, S. R., Zimm, C., Buxton, J. E., Bailey, E., Laybourn, L., Ghadiali, A., and Dyke, J. G. (Eds.): The Global Tipping Points Report 2023, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK, hal-04548845, 2023. 

Lindsay, R. W. and Zhang, J.: The Thinning of Arctic Sea Ice, 1988–2003: Have We Passed a Tipping Point?, J. Clim., 18, 4879–4894, https://doi.org/10.1175/JCLI3587.1, 2005. 

Lowe, T., Brown, K., Dessai, S., De França Doria, M., Haynes, K., and Vincent, K.: Does tomorrow ever come? Disaster narrative and public perceptions of climate change, Public. Underst. Sci., 15, 435–457, https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662506063796, 2006. 

Lujala, P., Lein, H., and Rød, J. K.: Climate change, natural hazards, and risk perception: the role of proximity and personal experience, Local Environ., 20, 489–509, https://doi.org/10.1080/13549839.2014.887666, 2015. 

Majid, U., Wasim, A., Bakshi, S., and Truong, J.: Knowledge, (mis-)conceptions, risk perception, and behavior change during pandemics: A scoping review of 149 studies, Public Underst. Sci., 29, 777–799, https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662520963365, 2020. 

Manzanedo, R. D. and Manning, P.: COVID-19: Lessons for the climate change emergency, Sci. Total Environ., 742, 140563, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.140563, 2020. 

Marx, S. M., Weber, E. U., Orlove, B. S., Leiserowitz, A., Krantz, D. H., Roncoli, C., and Phillips, J.: Communication and mental processes: Experiential and analytic processing of uncertain climate information, Glob. Environ. Change, 17, 47–58, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2006.10.004, 2007. 

Milfont, T. L.: The Interplay Between Knowledge, Perceived Efficacy, and Concern About Global Warming and Climate Change: A One-Year Longitudinal Study, Risk Anal., 32, 1003–1020, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01800.x, 2012. 

Milkoreit, M.: Science and climate change diplomacy: Cognitive limits and the need to reinvent science communication, Science Diplomacy, 109–131, https://doi.org/10.1142/9789814440073_0006, 2014. 

Milkoreit, M., Moore, M.-L., Schoon, M., and Meek, C. L.: Resilience scientists as change-makers – Growing the middle ground between science and advocacy?, Environ. Sci. Pol., 53, 87–95, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envsci.2014.08.003, 2015. 

Milkoreit, M.: Cognitive capacities for global governance in the face of complexity: The case of Climate tipping points, Global Challenges, Governance, and Complexity, 274–302, https://doi.org/10.4337/9781788115421.00023, 2019. 

Milkoreit, M., Hodbod, J., Baggio, J., Benessaiah, K., Calderón-Contreras, R., Donges, J. F., Mathias, J.-D., Rocha, J. C., Schoon, M., and Werners, S. E.: Defining tipping points for social-ecological systems scholarship – an interdisciplinary literature review, Environ. Res. Lett., 13, 033005, https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/aaaa75, 2018. 

Nadeau, C.: Missing the (tipping) point: the effect of information about climate tipping points on public risk perceptions in Norway, Zenodo [data set], https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13133016, 2024. 

Norgaard, K. M.: “We Don't Really Want to Know”: Environmental Justice and Socially Organized Denial of Global Warming in Norway, Organ. Environ., 19, 347–370, https://doi.org/10.1177/1086026606292571, 2006. 

Norgaard, K. M.: Living in denial: climate change, emotions, and everyday life, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, 279 pp., ISBN: 978-0-262-51585-6, 2011. 

Nuttall, M.: Tipping Points and the Human World: Living with Change and Thinking about the Future, AMBIO, 41, 96–105, https://doi.org/10.1007/s13280-011-0228-3, 2012. 

Nyborg, K., Anderies, J. M., Dannenberg, A., Lindahl, T., Schill, C., Schlüter, M., Adger, W. N., Arrow, K. J., Barrett, S., Carpenter, S., Chapin, F. S., Crépin, A.-S., Daily, G., Ehrlich, P., Folke, C., Jager, W., Kautsky, N., Levin, S. A., Madsen, O. J., Polasky, S., Scheffer, M., Walker, B., Weber, E. U., Wilen, J., Xepapadeas, A., and De Zeeuw, A.: Social norms as solutions, Science, 354, 42–43, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf8317, 2016. 

OECD: Climate Tipping Points: Insights for Effective Policy Action, OECD Publishing, Paris, https://doi.org/10.1787/abc5a69e-en, 2022 

Otto, I. M., Donges, J. F., Cremades, R., Bhowmik, A., Hewitt, R. J., Lucht, W., Rockström, J., Allerberger, F., McCaffrey, M., Doe, S. S. P., Lenferna, A., Morán, N., Van Vuuren, D. P., and Schellnhuber, H. J.: Social tipping dynamics for stabilizing Earth's climate by 2050, P. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 117, 2354–2365, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1900577117, 2020. 

Painter, J.: Climate change in the media: Reporting risk and uncertainty, Bloomsbury Publishing, I.B. Tauris & Co. Ltd., Tauris, London, ISBN: 9781780765884, 2013. 

Park, J. J.: Prototypes, Exemplars, and Theoretical and Applied Ethics, Neuroethics, 6, 237–247, https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-011-9106-8, 2013. 

Pereira, J. C. and Viola, E.: Catastrophic Climate Change and Forest Tipping Points: Blind Spots in International Politics and Policy, Glob. Policy, 9, 513–524, https://doi.org/10.1111/1758-5899.12578, 2018. 

PERITIA: Public perceptions on climate change, Kings College London, 2022. 

Plate, R.: Assessing individuals' understanding of nonlinear causal structures in complex systems, Syst. Dyn. Rev., 26, 19–33, https://doi.org/10.1002/sdr.432, 2010. 

Regjeringen: Vil videreutvikle oljeindustrien – forslag om Forbedret Petroleumsskatt, Regjeringen.no, https://www.regjeringen.no/no/aktuelt/ (last access: 14 January 2024), 2022. 

Renn, O.: The Systemic Risk Perspective: Social Perception of Uncertainty and Tipping Points, in: Strategies for Sustainability of the Earth System, edited by: Wilderer, P. A., Grambow, M., Molls, M., and Oexle, K., Springer International Publishing, Cham, 15–31, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-74458-8_2, 2022. 

Russill, C.: Climate change tipping points: Origins, precursors, and debates, WIREs Clim. Change, 6, 427–434, https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.344, 2015. 

Russill, C. and Nyssa, Z.: The tipping point trend in climate change communication, Glob. Environ. Change, 19, 336–344, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2009.04.001, 2009. 

Salas Reyes, R., Nguyen, V. M., Schott, S., Berseth, V., Hutchen, J., Taylor, J., and Klenk, N.: A Research Agenda for Affective Dimensions in Climate Change Risk Perception and Risk Communication, Front. Clim., 3, 751310, https://doi.org/10.3389/fclim.2021.751310, 2021. 

Scheffer, M., Bascompte, J., Brock, W. A., Brovkin, V., Carpenter, S. R., Dakos, V., Held, H., Van Nes, E. H., Rietkerk, M., and Sugihara, G.: Early-warning signals for critical transitions, Nature, 461, 53–59, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08227, 2009. 

Schellnhuber, H. J., Rahmstorf, S., and Winkelmann, R.: Why the right climate target was agreed in Paris, Nat. Clim. Change, 6, 649–653, https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate3013, 2016. 

Schweizer, P., Goble, R., and Renn, O.: Social Perception of Systemic Risks, Risk Analysis, 42, 1455–1471, https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13831, 2022. 

Sharpe, S. and Lenton, T. M.: Upward-scaling tipping cascades to meet climate goals: plausible grounds for hope, Clim. Policy, 21, 421–433, https://doi.org/10.1080/14693062.2020.1870097, 2021. 

Spence, A., Poortinga, W., and Pidgeon, N.: The psychological distance of climate change, Risk Anal., 32, 957–972, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2011.01695.x, 2012. 

Steffen, W., Rockström, J., Richardson, K., Lenton, T. M., Folke, C., Liverman, D., Summerhayes, C. P., Barnosky, A. D., Cornell, S. E., Crucifix, M., Donges, J. F., Fetzer, I., Lade, S. J., Scheffer, M., Winkelmann, R., and Schellnhuber, H. J.: Trajectories of the Earth System in the Anthropocene, P. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 115, 8252–8259, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1810141115, 2018. 

Sterman, J. D.: Communicating climate change risks in a skeptical world, Climatic Change, 108, 811–826, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-011-0189-3, 2011. 

Stoddard, I.: Three decades of climate mitigation: why haven't we bent the global emissions curve?, Ann. Rev. Environ. Res., 46, 653–689, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-environ-012220-011104, 2021. 

Thornalley, D. J. R., Oppo, D. W., Ortega, P., Robson, J. I., Brierley, C. M., Davis, R., Hall, I. R., Moffa-Sanchez, P., Rose, N. L., Spooner, P. T., Yashayaev, I., and Keigwin, L. D.: Anomalously weak Labrador Sea convection and Atlantic overturning during the past 150 years, Nature, 556, 227–230, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0007-4, 2018. 

van Beek, L., Milkoreit, M., Prokopy, L., Reed, J. B., Vervoort, J., Wardekker, A., and Weiner, R.: The effects of serious gaming on risk perceptions of climate tipping points, Climatic Change, 170, 31, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-022-03318-x, 2022. 

van der Hel, S., Hellsten, I., and Steen, G.: Tipping Points and Climate Change: Metaphor Between Science and the Media, Environ. Commun., 12, 605–620, https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2017.1410198, 2018. 

van der Linden, S.: The social-psychological determinants of climate change risk perceptions: Towards a comprehensive model, J. Environ. Psychol., 41, 112–124, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.11.012, 2015. 

van Valkengoed, A. M., Steg, L., and Perlaviciute, G.: The psychological distance of climate change is overestimated, One Earth, 6, 362–391, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.oneear.2023.03.006, 2023. 

Wang, S., Foster, A., Lenz, E. A., Kessler, J. D., Stroeve, J. C., Anderson, L. O., Turetsky, M., Betts, R., Zou, S., Liu, W., Boos, W. R., and Hausfather, Z.: Mechanisms and Impacts of Earth System Tipping Elements, Rev. Geophys., 61, e2021RG000757, https://doi.org/10.1029/2021RG000757, 2023. 

Weber, E. U.: Experience-Based and Description-Based Perceptions of Long-Term Risk: Why Global Warming does not Scare us (Yet), Climatic Change, 77, 103–120, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-006-9060-3, 2006. 

Winkelmann, R., Donges, J. F., Smith, E. K., Milkoreit, M., Eder, C., Heitzig, J., Katsanidou, A., Wiedermann, M., Wunderling, N., and Lenton, T. M.: Social tipping processes towards climate action: A conceptual framework, Ecol. Econ., 192, 107242, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2021.107242, 2022. 

Winton, M.: Does the Arctic sea ice have a tipping point?, Geophys. Res. Lett., 33, L23504, https://doi.org/10.1029/2006GL028017, 2006. 

WMO: WMO Global Annual to Decadal Climate Update (Target Years: 2023-2027), Geneva: World Meteorological Organization, https://library.wmo.int/index.php?lvl=notice_displayandid=22272 (last access: 15 June 2023), 2023 

Wunderling, N., Donges, J. F., Kurths, J., and Winkelmann, R.: Interacting tipping elements increase risk of climate domino effects under global warming, Earth Syst. Dynam., 12, 601–619, https://doi.org/10.5194/esd-12-601-2021, 2021. 

Wunderling, N., Winkelmann, R., Rockström, J., Loriani, S., Armstrong McKay, D. I., Ritchie, P. D. L., Sakschewski, B., and Donges, J. F.: Global warming overshoots increase risks of climate tipping cascades in a network model, Nat. Clim. Change, 13, 75–82, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-022-01545-9, 2023. 

Xie, B., Brewer, M. B., Hayes, B. K., McDonald, R. I., and Newell, B. R.: Predicting climate change risk perception and willingness to act, J. Environ. Psychol., 65, 101331, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101331, 2019.  

Yeganeh, A. J., McCoy, A. P., and Schenk, T.: Determinants of climate change policy adoption: A meta-analysis, Urban Clim., 31, 100547, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.uclim.2019.100547, 2020. 

YouGov: International poll: Most expect to feel impact of climate change, many think it will make us extinct, YouGov, https://d3nkl3psvxxpe9.cloudfront.net/ (last access: 15 March 2023), 2019. 

Download
Short summary

The paper examines how knowledge about climate tipping points (CTPs) influences public risk perceptions in Norway. Using an online survey, the study finds that only 13 % of Norwegians have good knowledge of climate tipping points. Communication about these tipping points had a modest effect, slightly increasing concern compared to general climate change information. The study highlights the need for further research on this topic, especially how to effectively communicate knowledge about CTPs.

Altmetrics
Final-revised paper
Preprint